Showing posts with label cognitive miserliness. Show all posts
Showing posts with label cognitive miserliness. Show all posts

Saturday, April 4, 2015

The information tsunami: Riding versus drowning

File:Great Wave off Kanagawa2.jpgA few things have come across my Twitter feed in recent weeks that relate to cognition and the Internet. The first is an article by Thomas Erren et al on 10 elements of lifelong learning according to Richard Hamming. I'm a big fan of Hamming's ideas and research philosophy, and the authors do a nice job of updating some important points from his book. I recommend reading the paper; to wet your appetite, the first two rules they describe include
Rule 1. Cultivate Lifelong Learning as a “Style of Thinking” That Concentrates on Fundamental Principles Rather Than on Facts
Rule 2. Structure Your Learning to Ride the Information Tsunami Rather Than Drown in It
These strike a chord when thought about in the context of the recent study by Barr et al, which suggested that smartphones, as entrée to vast stores of information, can supplant critical thinking by making it easy for people to offload thinking to technology. Hamming, Erren et al, and Barr et al collectively remind us of the dangers of merely looking for facts on the Internet as opposed to concentrating on forming a coherent body of knowledge out of those facts.

Eric Topol captures this perfectly in a recent tweet:
The future of medicine is not about looking things up on the Internet; it's being able to generate one's own real world data+super-analytics
I couldn't agree more. Data and "super-analytics" need to be aimed at generating and conveying a coherent picture of health so that consumers of those analytics -- whether researchers, physicians, or non-specialists -- are informed and educated.

As the sea of facts gets larger and more accessible, becoming a tsunami, we face the risk of drowning, as Erren et al suggest, or becoming cognitively lazy, as Barr et al suggest. How do humans synthesize knowledge from data, especially when the data may be messy, variable, or uncertain? In the case of public health, the issue is hugely important, because people will synthesize their own knowledge based on facts they find compelling (e.g., the University of Google, "My science is named Evan"). How do we leverage the information superhighway to produce insight and decisions grounded in the relevant facts?

(image source: Wikipedia)

Saturday, March 14, 2015

The calculus of online credibility

It's been estimated that roughly one quarter of the global population will soon be using smartphones. Recently, Nathaniel Barr et al have studied how the instant and ubiquitous access to information from smartphone use is impacting our propensity for critical thinking. The paper's abstract captures the issue:
With the advent of Smartphone technology, access to the Internet and its associated knowledge base is at one’s fingertips. What consequences does this have for human cognition? We frame Smartphone use as an instantiation of the extended mind—the notion that our cognition goes beyond our brains—and in so doing, characterize a modern form of cognitive miserliness. Specifically, that people typically forego effortful analytic thinking in lieu of fast and easy intuition suggests that individuals may allow their Smartphones to do their thinking for them. Our account predicts that individuals who are relatively less willing and/or able to engage effortful reasoning processes may compensate by relying on the Internet through their Smartphones. . . . These findings demonstrate that people may offload thinking to technology, which in turn demands that psychological science understand the meshing of mind and media to adequately characterize human experience and cognition in the modern era.
Tania Lombrozo, writing for NPR, put it more succinctly:
We all know a little knowledge can be a dangerous thing. Research increasingly supports a related proposition — that easy knowledge can be a dangerous thing.
I don't want to go into the strengths and weaknesses of the study, or it's ability to assess causality. Researchers will no doubt address these important issues in time, and Lombrozo discusses them nicely in her essay. However, it's important to contemplate some of the implications of the study.

One implication is that, as the Internet and mobile technology have changed the way we acquire information, they have also changed the way in which we assess its credibility. We've all seen people read claims and discussions from different online media and rapidly accept them as true. Sometimes the information they glean is true -- online communities of people possessing similar interests and expertise are often rich sources of specialized information -- whereas other times online discourse is not so authoritative. Regardless, acceptance of what surfers read is often rapid. If people increasingly rely on digital information and forgo complex, analytic thinking -- the "cognitive miserliness" that Barr et al describe -- perhaps it's because they trust what they're reading.

If true, what are the implications for public health? Insofar as health behavior is influenced through online information gathering, we must understand how people determine trust and credibility of online information. Professional achievement in terms of academic degrees, certifications, or job titles of those publishing information online may be important, but also important are a source's online profile in terms of site appearance and ease of navigation, number of followers, and number of page views. In fact, the latter may be more important than the former.

If it's true that we are increasingly putting our brains in our pockets and avoiding critical thinking, as Barr et al suggest, then it's important to understand the calculus of credibility in cyberspace. If people find Facebook, Reddit threads, and Fox News more credible than the CDC or their own doctor -- rightly or wrongly -- we had better understand why and engage it as a tool.

(image source: David Hartley)